Y. Gunnar-declerck-coello and O. Iwanow, Effect of structuring the workspace on cognitive and sensorimotor distance estimation : no dissociation between perception and action, Perception and Psychophysics, pp.278-289, 2006.

A. Cowey, M. Small, and S. Ellis, Left visuo-spatial neglect can be worse in far than in near space, Neuropsychologia, vol.32, issue.9, pp.1-6, 1994.
DOI : 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90152-X

J. E. Cutting and P. M. Vishton, Perceiving layout and knowing distances : The integration, relative potency, and contextual use of different information about depth, dans Perceiving space and motion, édité par Epstein, pp.69-117, 1995.

D. Zeh, On the interpretation of measurement in quantum theory, Foundations of Physics, vol.202, issue.1, pp.69-76
DOI : 10.1007/BF00708656

P. W. Halligan, . Marshall, and C. John, Left neglect for near but not far space in man, Nature, vol.350, issue.6318, pp.498-500
DOI : 10.1038/350498a0

S. Hawking, The edge of space time traduction française « Le bord de l'espace temps, Gallimard, pp.61-69, 1989.

E. Husserl, Leçons pour une phénoménologie de la conscience intime du temps, 1905.

C. Lenay and P. Steiner, Beyond the internalism/externalism debate: The constitution of the space of perception, Consciousness and Cognition, vol.19, issue.4, pp.938-952, 2010.
DOI : 10.1016/j.concog.2010.06.011

M. R. Longo and S. F. Lourenco, On the nature of near space: Effects of tool use and the transition to far space, Neuropsychologia, vol.44, issue.6, pp.977-981, 2006.
DOI : 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.09.003

. Magnan, Christian 2005 La nature sans foi ni loi

G. Declerck-metzger-james, J. Gibson, R. B. Macleod, and H. L. Pick, Wolfgang 1974 Can the subject create his world ?, dans Perception Essays in Honor of, pp.57-71

U. Mohrhoff, Quantum Physics (quant-ph), 2001.

O. 'regan, J. K. Noë, and A. , A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness, Behavioral and Brain Sciences, vol.24, issue.05, pp.939-973
DOI : 10.1017/S0140525X01000115

É. Pacherie, Du problème de Molyneux au problème de Bach-y-Rita, dans Perception et Intermodalité, Approches actuelles du Problème de Molyneux, édité par Proust, pp.255-293, 1997.
DOI : 10.3917/puf.prous.1997.01.0253

URL : https://jeannicod.ccsd.cnrs.fr/ijn_00000242/document

. Poincaré, Henri 1902 La Science et l'Hypothèse, Paris : Flammarion, 1968.

D. R. Proffitt, J. Stefanucci, T. Banton, and W. Epstein, The role of effort in perceiving distance, Psychological Science, vol.2, issue.4, pp.106-113, 2003.
DOI : 10.1037//0096-1523.21.3.480

D. R. Proffitt, Embodied Perception and the Economy of Action, Perspectives on Psychological Science, vol.31, issue.2, pp.110-122, 2006.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.9.3.427

R. Ruyer and . Schrödinger, Erwin 1951 Physique quantique et représentation du monde, Paris : Éditions du Seuil, chap, Science et humanisme, pp.19-87

. Sedgwick, A. R. Harold, K. L. Boff, J. P. Kaufman, and . Thomas, 1986 Space perception, dans Handbook of Perception and Human Performance, pp.128-167

P. Vuilleumier, N. Valenza, E. Mayer, A. Reverdin, and T. Landis, Near and far visual space in unilateral neglect, Annals of Neurology, vol.386, issue.3, pp.406-410, 1998.
DOI : 10.1002/ana.410430324

J. K. Witt, D. R. Proffitt, and W. Epstein, Tool Use Affects Perceived Distance, But Only When You Intend to Use It., Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, vol.31, issue.5, pp.31-880, 2005.
DOI : 10.1037/0096-1523.31.5.880